Humanity : Heedless (Care-less)
Care·less
People have been saying they could care less, when,
logically, they mean they couldn’t care less.
The phrase "I couldn’t care less" originated in
Britain and made its way to the United States in the 1950s. The phrase "I
could care less" appeared in the US about a decade later.
In 1990s, the well-known Harvard
professor and language writer Stephen Pinker argued that the way most people
say could care less — implies they are being ironic or sarcastic.
Regardless of the reason people say they could care less, it
is one of the more common language. To say “care less means you has a bit of
caring left”, which is not what seem the intention of the speaker. The proper
"couldn’t care less" is still the dominant form, but "could care
less" has been steadily gaining ground since its appearance in the 1960s.
Definitions:
2. Marked
by or resulting from lack of forethought or thoroughness : a careless mistake.
3. Showing
a lack of consideration : a careless remark.
4. Unconcerned
or indifferent; heedless : careless of the consequences.
5. Unstudied
or effortless : dancing with careless grace.
6. Exhibiting a disposition that is
free from cares; cheerful l: a careless grin; a careless wave of the hand.
HUMANS ARE UNLIKELY to win the world’s prize for the fastest, the strongest or the largest, but are world champions at understanding one another. This interpersonal mastery is fueled, partly, by empathy: the tendency to care about and share other people’s emotional experiences.
Empathy
is a prowess of human behavior and has been considered a natural ability. A
study, however, found that empathy levels have been declining over the past 30
years.
The
research, led by Sara H. Konrath of the University of Michigan at Ann Arbor and
published in August in Personality
and Social Psychology Review, found that college students’
self-reported empathy has declined since 1980, with drop in the past 10 years.
During the same period students’ self-reported self-obsession has reached a new
height, according to research by Jean M. Twenge, a psychologist at San Diego
State University.
An
individual’s empathy can be assessed in many ways, but the most popular is
simply asking people what they think of themselves.
The
Interpersonal Reactivity Index, a well-known questionnaire, taps empathy with
statements such as:
a.
“I
often have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me” and
b.
“I try to look at everybody’s side of a
disagreement before I make a decision.”
People
vary greatly on the ideal of how empathic they consider themselves. Researcher
confirms when someone are acting empathically, they are actually demonstrating their
empathy in a visible ways, ranging from mimicking others’ postures to helping
people in need (for example, offering to take notes for a sick fellow student).
Ever
since the inceptions of Interpersonal Reactivity Index in 1979, thousands of
students had solicited their views on their empathetic behavior. Konrath and
her colleagues took advantages of data by collating self-reported empathy
scores of nearly 14,000 students. Then using a technique known as
cross-temporal meta-analysis to measure whether scores have changed over the years.
The
results were startling: almost 75 percent of students today rate themselves as
less empathic than the average student 30 years ago.
What’s to Blame?
This
information seems to conflict with studies suggesting that empathy is a trait
people are born with. A 2007 study by Yale University developmental
psychologists found that six-month-old infants demonstrate an association for
empathic behavior, preferring simple dolls they have seen helping others over
visually similar bullies. At the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary
Anthropology in Leipzig have shown that without persuasions and incentives,
toddlers help and share rewards with others. Empathic behavior is not confined
to humans or even to primates. In a recent study mice reacted more strongly to
painful stimuli when they saw another mouse suffering, suggesting that they
“share” the pain of their cage mates.
However
new finding found that empathy is on the decline indicating that even when human
trait is ingrained, social connections can exert a profound effect, changing
even our most basic emotional responses. Precisely what is eroding young people
of their natural impulse to feel for others remains mysterious, however,
because scientists cannot design a study to evaluate changes that occurred in
the past. As Twenge puts it, “you can’t randomly assign people to a
generation.” There are various theories.
a. Social Isolations
Konrath
cites with increase in social isolation, coincided with the drop in empathy. In
the past 30 years Americans have become more likely to live alone and less
likely to join groups—ranging from PTAs to political parties to casual sports
teams. This type of isolation can take a toll on people’s attitudes toward
others. Steve Duck of the University of Iowa has found that socially isolated,
as compared with integrated, individuals evaluate others less generously after
interacting with them, and Kenneth J. Rotenberg of Keele University in England
has shown that lonely people are more likely to take advantage of others’ trust
to cheat them in laboratory games.
b. Information
The types of information we consume
have also shifted in recent decades; specifically, Americans have abandoned
reading. The number of adults who read literature for pleasure sank below 50
percent for the first time ever in the past 10 years, with the decrease
occurring most sharply among college-age adults. And reading may be linked to
empathy. In a study published by psychologist Raymond A. Mar of York University
in Toronto and others demonstrated that the number of stories preschoolers read
predicts their ability to understand the emotions of others. Mar has also shown
that adults who read less fiction report themselves to be less empathic.
c.
Social
Status
Turning a blind eye. Giving someone
the cold shoulder. Looking down on people. Seeing right through them.
This behavior suggested,
the social distance between those with greater power and those with less, have
an impact on the sphere of interpersonal interactions and may aggravate the
soaring inequality.
In a recent research, shows that people with the most social
power pay scant attention to those with little of such power. It has been
observed, for instance, a mere five-minute get-acquainted session with
strangers, the more powerful person shows fewer signals of paying attention,
like nodding or laughing. Higher-status people are also more likely to express
disregard, through facial expressions, and are more likely to take over the
conversation and interrupt or look past the other speaker.
Of course, in any society, social power is relative; any of
us may be higher or lower in a given interaction, and the research shows the
effect still prevails. Though the more powerful pay less attention to us than
we do to them, in other situations we are relatively higher on the totem pole
of status — and we, too, tend to pay less attention to those a rung or two
down.
d. Sufferings
A prerequisite to empathy is simply paying attention to the
person in pain. In 2008, social psychologists from the University of Amsterdam
and the University of California, Berkeley, studied pairs of strangers telling
one another about difficulties they had been through, like a divorce or death
of a loved one. The researchers found that the different expression in the
playing down of suffering. The more powerful were less compassionate toward the
hardships described by the less powerful.
e. Politics
In politics, it is readily notions that politicians have the
notions of dismissing inconvenient people to the extent of dismissing the inconvenient
truths about them. The insistence by some politician on cutting financing for
food stamps and impeding the implementation of care, which would allow
patients, including those with pre-existing health conditions, to obtain and
pay for insurance coverage, may stem partly from the empathy gap. As political
scientists have noted, redistricting and gerrymandering have led to the
creation of more and more safe districts, in which elected officials don’t even
have to encounter many voters from the rival party, much less empathize with
them.
f. Prejudice
Freud called this “the narcissism of minor differences,” a
theme repeated by Vamik D. Volkan,
an emeritus professor of psychiatry at the University of Virginia, who was born
in Cyprus to Turkish parents. As a small boy, Dr. Volkan remembers hearing negatively
about the hated Greek Cypriots — is actually
share many similarities with Turkish Cypriots. Yet for decades their
modest-size island has been politically divided, which magnify the problem by
letting prejudicial myths flourish.
g.
Valuable
Mr. Keltner suggests that, in general, we only focus the
most on those we value most. While the wealthy can hire help, those with few
material assets are more likely to value their social assets: like the neighbor
who will keep an eye on your child from the time she gets home from school
until the time you get home from work. The financial difference ends up
creating a behavioral difference. Poor people are better attuned to
interpersonal relations — with those of the same strata, and the more powerful
— than the rich are, because they have to be.
While Mr. Keltner’s research finds that the poor, compared
with the wealthy, have keenly attuned interpersonal attention in all
directions, in general, those with the most power in society seem to pay
particularly little attention to those with the least power. To be sure,
high-status people do attend to those of equal rank — but not as well as those
low of status do.
h. Interpersonal Contact
In contrast, extensive interpersonal contact counteracts any
biases by letting people from hostile groups get to know one another as
individuals and even friends. Thomas F. Pettigrew,
a research professor of social psychology at the University of California,
Santa Cruz, analyzed more than 500 studies on intergroup contact.
In his research, he found that even in areas where ethnic
groups conflict and viewed negatively, individuals who had close friends within
the other group exhibited little or no such prejudice. They seemed to realize
the many ways those demonized “others” were “just like me.” Whether such
friendly social contact would overcome the divide between those with more and
less social and economic power was not studied, but I suspect it would help.
Whereas the sources of empathic decline are impossible to
pinpoint, the work of Konrath and Twenge demonstrates that the American
personality is shifting in an ominous direction. Still, we are not doomed to
become a society of self-obsessed loners. Konrath points out that if life
choices can drive empathy down, then making different choices could nurture it.
“The fact that empathy is declining means that there’s more fluidity to it than
previously thought,” she says. “It means that empathy can change. It can go
up.”
Responsibility Deficit Disorder?
According
to Dr. Sapadin, a psychologist, author and success coach. Some people are simply irresponsible.
a.
They may be careless and unpredictable or
outright reckless.
b.
They “forget” about appointments.
c.
They’re chronically late.
d.
They neglect to plan ahead.
e.
They’re financially irresponsible.
f.
They don’t take care of their stuff.
g.
They make rash decisions that get them into
trouble.
h.
They ignore deadlines.
i.
They act as though others should bail them
out of whatever trouble they get into.
This
people are not all adolescents but human adults. It could be a friend, a family
member or a colleague. We may love them yet we experiencing them can be terribly frustrating. We want to shake them.
Yell at them. Knock some sense into their brains. But none of this seems to
make a difference to them. They shrug it all off.
Why?
Because they have Responsibility Deficit Disorder (RDD), a much-needed
diagnostic category that I have just created.
RDD
is prevalent in our society and is a growing problem. Those who have it do not
“suffer” from it. Quite the contrary. The people who “suffer” are those loved
ones who must deal with the rat’s nest that is so often dropped in their laps.
Responsibility is a Choice
More Responsible
|
Less Responsible
|
Facts, realism,
reality, and learning what is.
Inquiring, investigating, seeking, and embracing facts and truth. Reason and
sanity.
|
Fantasy. Avoiding
reality; embracing fantasy or magical thinking. Denying or escaping reality.
Rumors. Rejecting facts. Insanity.
|
Honesty.
|
Dishonesty.
|
Focusing our
thoughts and attention. Orderly.
|
Drifting, rambling,
unfocused, flighty, and chaotic.
|
Thinking through
alternatives and consequences.
|
Relying on habit or
taking the easy way.
|
Clear and
consistent thinking and expression.
|
Obscurity, vagueness,
hedging, inconsistency, waffling.
|
Learn from history.
|
Revise, ignore, or
dispute history.
|
Seeking out
expertise.
|
Dismissing
expertise.
|
Working to
understand.
|
Whatever.
|
Self-discipline.
Impulse control. Behaving true to your values. Saying “yes” to your values
and “no” to the rest.
|
Acting on impulse.
Going along with whatever. Situational ethics.
|
Do what you say.
|
Do whatever.
|
Fulfill reciprocal
obligations.
|
Evade reciprocal
obligations. Cheat.
|
Open to new ideas
and information. Curious. Wise.
|
Closed to new
ideas. Stubborn and closed minded. Ignorant.
|
Willing to accept blame for errors.
|
Infallible,
arrogant, dismissive, obstinate.
|
Consistent,
congruent, and reliable.
|
Inconsistent,
chaotic, unreliable.
|
Rationality,
valuing reason, respect for facts, and valid logic. Gathering, validating,
and studying evidence.
Developing and applying a coherent theory of
knowledge.
|
Fallacies, distortions,
assumptions, misinformation, and unrepresentative data.
|
Considering a variety
of points-of-view.
|
Accepting a
one-sided view.
|
Rigorous, careful,
attentive.
|
Sloppy, careless,
distracted.
|
Adaptation and
flexibility. Adjusting beliefs and actions to accommodate newly understood
facts.
|
Rigid and misfit.
|
Competence.
|
Manipulation.
|
Dependence.
|
|
Courage.
|
Cowardice.
|
Seeking solutions.
|
Assigning blame.
|
Integrity.
|
|
Awareness and wisdom.
|
Unawareness and ignorance.
|
Adherence to
evidence, values, and choice.
|
Blind obedience.
|
Entrepreneurship.
|
Bureaucracy
|
Participant.
|
Bystander.
|
Entitled to my own
opinion.
|
Entitled to my own
facts.
|
Talking to people.
|
Talking about people.
|
Building enduring
relationships based on who we are.
|
Seeking
instrumental relationships based on what we do.
|
I choose to . . .
I decided to . . . |
I had to . . .
I had no choice . . . |
Ideas, choices, and
actions do matter and do have consequences. Believing that effects and
outcomes have causes.
|
It's all up to
fate, destiny, and chance. Attributing results to chance or destiny. There is
nothing I can do; I am helpless.
|
Internal Locus of
Control
|
External Locus of
Control.
|
Combating Care less
If
all this sounds familiar to you, here’s what you must do to save your own
sanity.
- Control impulses and act deliberately according to their values, well-chosen beliefs, and long term goals,
- Consider the needs of others and the community, not only themselves,
- Are generous rather than selfish, kind rather than cruel, gratified, not greedy,
- Are comfortable with complexity, doubt, and ambiguity, they are not quick to judge,
- Control their emotions and don't tolerate tantrums, anger displays, self-indulgence, and violence,
- Are emotionally competent and apply a robust theory of knowledge,
- Integrate experiences and information to act rationally, consistently, and reliably rather than unpredictably, inconsistently, irrationally, and erratically. Adults are stable, even tempered, and non-volatile.
- Are patient and consider the long term, not only this fleeting moment,
- Speak with candor and don't tell lies, speak disingenuously, or mislead,
- Are trustworthy, not manipulative; respect others and play by a fair set of rules. Children often cheat and expect to win at any cost.
- Choose wisdom over ignorance,
- Value reason over power, the pen over the sword,
- Confront problems and transcend conflict, rather than deny and avoid problems, instigate quarrels, become vindictive, or seek revenge,
- Accept responsibility for their actions, admit mistakes, accept their share of the blame, and apologize to others,
- Accept and assimilate facts, rather than dismiss, distort, ignore, spin, self-justify, or fantasize,
- Maintain a balanced perspective; adults tolerate trivial transgressions while courageously upholding the most vital principles,
- Attain an authentic humility and keep their egos in check.
- Are authentic, not phony,
- Are autonomous, competent, and value their interdependence with others,
- Are helpful, not helpless.
- Are sober, not strung-out,
- Enjoy fun, but never at the expense of others.
Our world requires adult supervision;
take responsibility to act your age. Children choose easy over hard, simplistic
over complex, and fast over slow. But significant contributions are often
difficult, complex, and slow to achieve. Forego the cheap thrills to achieve
satisfaction and significance. You are a competent, autonomous adult. You are fully responsible for all your
words and actions, as are other competent adults; it is time to put
away childish things.
Extracts
and excerpt taken with many thanks from :
· Daniel
Goleman, a psychologist, is the author of “Emotional Intelligence” and, most
recently, “Focus: The Hidden Driver of Excellence.”
Linda Sapadin, Ph.Dhttp://psychcentral.com/blog/archives/2013/04/03/do-you-know-someone-with-responsibility-deficit-disorder/
· http://www.emotionalcompetency.com/responsibility.htm
Comments
Post a Comment